Evidence Summary

Iron Deficiency Anemia in Young Children: Screening

March 30, 2015

Recommendations made by the USPSTF are independent of the U.S. government. They should not be construed as an official position of the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality or the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.

By Marian S. McDonagh, PharmD, Ian Blazina, MPH, Tracy Dana, MLS, Amy Cantor, MD, MPH, Christina Bougatsos, MPH

The information in this article is intended to help clinicians, employers, policymakers, and others make informed decisions about the provision of health care services. This article is intended as a reference and not as a substitute for clinical judgment.

This article may be used, in whole or in part, as the basis for the development of clinical practice guidelines and other quality enhancement tools, or as a basis for reimbursement and coverage policies. AHRQ or U.S. Department of Health and Human Services endorsement of such derivative products may not be stated or implied.

This article was published online first at www.pediatrics.org on March 31, 2015.

Return to Table of Contents

Background and Objectives: Supplementation and screening for iron-deficiency anemia (IDA) in young children may improve growth and development outcomes. The goal of this study was to review the evidence regarding the benefits and harms of screening and routine supplementation for IDA for the U.S. Preventive Services Task Force.

Methods: We searched Medline and Cochrane databases (1996–August 2014), as well as reference lists of relevant systematic reviews. We included trials and controlled observational studies regarding the effectiveness and harms of routine iron supplementation and screening in children ages 6 to 24 months conducted in developed countries. One author extracted data, which were checked for accuracy by a second author. Dual quality assessment was performed.

Results: No studies of iron supplementation in young children reported on the diagnosis of neurodevelopmental delay. Five of 6 trials sparsely reporting various growth outcomes found no clear benefit of supplementation. After 3 to 12 months, Bayley Scales of Infant Development scores were not significantly different in 2 trials. Ten trials assessing iron supplementation in children reported inconsistent findings for hematologic measures. Evidence regarding the harms of supplementation was limited but did not indicate significant differences. No studies assessed the benefits or harms of screening or the association between improvement in impaired iron status and clinical outcomes. Studies may have been underpowered, and control factors varied and could have confounded results.

Conclusion: Although some evidence on supplementation for IDA in young children indicates improvements in hematologic values, evidence on clinical outcomes is lacking. No randomized controlled screening studies are available.

Return to Table of Contents

Iron-deficiency anemia (IDA), defined as iron deficiency (serum ferritin <12 μg/L) with hemoglobin levels <110 g/L,1, 2 can present a significant burden of disease in infancy and childhood. Iron is required in the production of hemoglobin, an essential protein found in red blood cells, and is stored in the body for use in hemoglobin production. Iron deficiency occurs when the level of stored iron becomes depleted. IDA occurs when iron levels are sufficiently depleted to produce anemia, characterized by hypochromic microcytic red blood cells.3 Although infants in the United States with iron deficiency are usually asymptomatic, IDA has been associated in some observational studies with cognitive and behavioral delays in children. However, these studies had methodologic flaws;4 for example, the outcomes examined were varied and not clearly clinically important. The effect of IDA in infancy and childhood has been reported in few well-designed, long-term controlled studies.

Iron deficiency among infants and toddlers in the United States has a prevalence of about 8% in the general population;5–7 however, only about one third of children who are iron deficient have associated anemia.5, 8, 9 The prevalence of IDA in children between the ages of 1 and 5 years is estimated to be about 1% to 2% in the United States.8, 10 The prevalence in children from low-income families is estimated to be slightly higher (i.e., about 3% for boys and 4% for girls based on 1 study of 432 one- to three-year-old children residing in California7). Current evidence regarding the prevalence of IDA in infants <1 year old in the United States is lacking, although estimates for low-risk infants in other developed countries range from 2% to 4%.11,12 Screening young children for IDA may lead to earlier identification and therefore earlier treatment, which has the potential to prevent negative health outcomes. However, the advent of iron fortification in the United States in many children's food products may influence our current understanding of IDA.

In 2006, the U.S. Preventive Services Task Force (USPSTF) concluded that the evidence was insufficient to recommend for or against routine screening for IDA or routine iron supplementation for asymptomatic children aged 6 to 12 months who are at average risk for IDA (I statement).13 These recommendations were based on a lack of evidence that screening resulted in improved health outcomes, as well as poor and conflicting evidence regarding the benefit of iron supplementation in children who are not at increased risk of IDA. At that time, the USPSTF recommended routine iron supplementation for asymptomatic children aged 6 to 12 months who are at increased risk for IDA (B recommendation), based on evidence that iron supplementation may improve neurodevelopmental outcomes in children who are at increased risk of IDA, which outweighs any potential harms.

The present review was commissioned by the USPSTF to update the previous recommendations.13 The scope of this review includes evidence regarding the benefits and harms of routine iron supplementation, screening for IDA in children ages 6 to 24 months, and the association between a change in iron status and improvement in child health outcomes in populations relevant to the United States.

Return to Table of Contents

Detailed methods and data for this review (including search strategies, inclusion criteria, abstraction and quality rating tables, information on risk factors and risk assessment tools, and results related to biochemical and composite intermediate outcomes) are provided in the full report.14 The protocol was developed by using a standardized process15 with input from experts and the public. In consultation with the USPSTF, analytic frameworks and Key Questions were developed for routine supplementation (Supplemental Appendix Figure 2) and for screening for IDA (Supplemental Appendix Figure 3) to show the linkages between Key Questions and bodies of evidence.

A research librarian searched the Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials, the Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (through the second quarter, 2014), and Medline (1996–August 2014) for relevant studies to update the previous USPSTF reviews.16,17 Because the previous research focused on systematic reviews and key studies of treatments for IDA, we also searched the reference lists of systematic reviews18–20 to identify any additional, relevant studies published before 1996.

Studies were selected on the basis of inclusion and exclusion criteria developed for each Key Question. Articles were selected for full review if they were related to IDA in children who received an intervention (supplementation or screening and related treatment) between the ages of 6 and 24 months. We restricted inclusion to English-language articles and excluded studies published only as abstracts. For all Key Questions, the focus was on studies that involved iron supplementation and treatment regimens commonly used in clinical practice in the United States. We excluded studies conducted in resource-poor populations, including nutritionally deficient populations in developing countries and populations in areas expected to have a high prevalence of hemoparasites, by selecting studies conducted in countries listed as having "high" or "very high" human development based on the international United Nations Human Development Index.21 At least 2 reviewers independently evaluated each study to determine eligibility.

Clinical outcomes of study were morbidity (including growth; cognitive, psychomotor, and neurodevelopmental outcomes; and diagnosis of developmental delay), mortality, and quality of life. Harm outcomes included accidental overdose, study discontinuations, and other harms related to screening, supplementation, or treatment. Included intermediate outcomes were incidence of IDA, iron deficiency, and anemia, as well as hematologic indices such as ferritin levels. Randomized controlled trials, nonrandomized controlled clinical trials, and controlled cohort studies were included for all Key Questions.

Details about the study design, patient population, setting, screening method, interventions, analysis, follow-up, and results were abstracted. A second investigator reviewed the data abstraction for accuracy. Two investigators independently applied criteria developed by the USPSTF15 to rate the internal validity (quality) of each study as good, fair, or poor. Discrepancies were resolved through a consensus process. When otherwise not reported and where possible, relative risks (RRs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) or P values were calculated.

The aggregate quality of the body of evidence for each Key Question (i.e., good, fair, poor) was assessed by using methods developed by the USPSTF; these assessments were based on the number, quality and size of studies; consistency of results between studies; and directness of evidence.15 Meta-analysis was not attempted due to the limited number of studies for each Key Question and differences among studies in design, population, and outcomes.

Return to Table of Contents

Figure 1 shows the results of the literature search and selection process.

Routine Iron Supplementation

Benefits of Routine Iron Supplementation in Children Ages 6 to 24 Months

A 1996 review conducted for the USPSTF17 found adequate evidence that iron prophylaxis resulted in reductions in the incidence of iron deficiency and IDA, but few data focusing on clinical outcomes were reported. The 2006 update16 did not assess the effect of supplementation on intermediate outcomes, and it found mixed evidence regarding the benefit of iron supplementation on neurodevelopmental test scores.

Overall, 10 trials of iron supplementation were included in this update.22–34 One study was rated as good quality,29 7 as fair quality,22–28, 32 and 2 as poor quality.30, 31 In general, children were enrolled between 6 and 9 months of age. Iron supplementation was administered for durations ranging from 3 to 18 months. Supplementation was provided as oral iron drops, iron-fortified formula, and as iron-fortified milk, foods, or meat. Controls used in the studies varied, and included a non–iron-containing formula or supplement, a specific diet, cow's milk, or nothing. Race or ethnicity was poorly reported. Enrolled sample sizes ranged from 24 to 493, except for 1 larger study of 1798 children; many studies analyzed fewer numbers due to loss to followup or refusal to undergo venipuncture. Only 1 study analyzed children on an intention-to-treat basis;29 the proportion of the sample available for analysis at the end of the other studies ranged from 53% to 92%. Most studies excluded children born prematurely and those with conditions likely to affect iron absorption, growth, or development, thus ruling out some children at higher than average risk for IDA but not specifically targeting those at average risk.

Methodologic shortcomings included unclear methods of randomization and allocation concealment,25–27 ,30–32 lack of or unclear methods of blinding,22, 23, 25, 27, 30, 31 and high or differential loss to follow-up.24, 27, 28, 30, 31 In addition, studies may be underpowered; although 7 studies reported some power or sample size calculations, they were limited to certain outcomes and varying differences in effect sizes.24, 26–29, 31, 32 For example, 2 studies were reported to be powered for developmental outcomes, with 1 sufficient to detect a 5-point difference on the Bayley Scales of Infant Development28 and 1 sufficient to detect a 2.5-point difference in "developmental scores" (scale not mentioned).26 We did not pool the results because of the heterogeneity of the studies in terms of supplementation method, dose, duration, timing of initiation and follow-up, and methodologic limitations. In addition, risk factors were largely not reported, and no studies stratified results according to risk groups.

Six fair-quality placebo-controlled trials of routine iron supplementation in young children sparsely reported various growth outcomes; 5 trials found no clear effect of supplementation on weight, length, or head circumference after 3 to 12 months of follow-up (Table 1).22, 25, 26, 28–30 As noted earlier, studies may have been underpowered to detect growth outcomes. Most sample sizes varied from 70 to 428, with 1 study including 1657 children. The only study reporting statistically significant differences in growth parameters found lesser growth values in the iron-supplemented group, possibly due to baseline differences in these growth outcomes.26 The group that received iron began the study with lesser values (weight: 7.98 vs. 8.09 kg; length: 66.6 vs. 66.9 cm [both P < 0.01]). Although this study was the largest, it was conducted in Chile, had a high incidence of IDA in the control group (22.6%), and suffered from methodologic flaws. Children were initially randomized to receive low or high-iron supplementation, but the low-iron intervention was replaced with a no-iron intervention partially through the study, partly because the interim analysis suggested that the low-iron condition was sufficient to prevent IDA. For analysis, all children who received any iron supplementation were combined and compared with children who did not receive supplementation, breaking randomization and leading to baseline differences between the groups. Because randomization was broken, we viewed the results as a fair-quality, comparative observational study. The authors found the following significant results for the iron-supplemented versus no-iron groups, respectively, after 12 months: weight, 10.0 vs. 10.1 kg (P < 0.05); length, 74.7 vs. 75.1 cm (P < 0.001); length for age (z score), –0.27 vs. –0.15 (P < 0.01); and head circumference, 46.7 vs. 47.0 cm (P < 0.001); the changes were significant possibly because the iron-supplemented group had lower values at baseline. Other clinical outcomes, such as diagnosis of psychomotor or neurodevelopmental delay or quality of life, were not reported in any trial.

Although not clearly clinical outcomes, developmental test scores after follow-up periods of 3 to 12 months were reported in 3 fair-quality trials.23, 26, 28 Two trials (n = 428 and 1657, respectively), including the Chilean study with methodologic flaws mentioned earlier, found no statistically significant difference between groups on the Bayley Scales of Infant Development (Table 2). Differences between groups were small and ranged from 0.6 to 0.7 on mental development and 0.2 to 0.7 for psychomotor development.26, 28 One trial of children potentially at higher risk for IDA used the Griffiths scale to measure psychomotor development.23 Although scores in both groups declined and were within normal limits at 24 months, they declined less in the iron-supplemented group (general quotient score at 24 months: −9.3 vs. −14.7; P = 0.04).

Ten trials of iron supplementation in children reported inconsistent findings for incidence of IDA, iron deficiency, and anemia, as well as changes in hemoglobin and serum ferritin (Table 1) (poor-quality studies were omitted from tables).22, 24–32

Iron supplementation was not found to influence the incidence of IDA in 4 good- or fair-quality studies (n values ranged from 62 to 284).24, 26, 27, 29, 32 One study, the aforementioned larger Chilean study (n = 1657) with a high incidence of IDA in the control group (22.6%) and methodologic flaws regarding randomization, reported a significant benefit (RR, 0.14 [95% CI, 0.09–0.20]).26 Overall, the studies reported ranges of IDA from 0% to 8% for those in the supplementation group and 0% to 22.6% in the placebo group.

For incidence of iron deficiency, 2 fair-quality studies with high incidences in the control groups suggest a significant benefit of supplementation. These findings included 1 study (n = 302) that compared iron-fortified formula with non–iron-fortified formula and cow's milk (6% vs. 22% vs. 43%)24 and the large (n = 1657) Chilean study (26.5% vs. 51.3%; RR, 0.52 [95% CI, 0.45–0.59]).26 Three other studies found no difference in rates of iron deficiency,24, 27, 30 including the only study conducted in the United States (RR, 0.94 [95% CI, 0.74–1.20]).24 Overall, the studies reported rates of iron deficiency ranging from 3.9% to 78% for those in the supplementation group and from 7.7% to 84% in the placebo group.

Six trials (5 fair-quality and 1 poor-quality; n values ranged from 62 to 1657) reported the effect of supplementation on the rate of anemia (variably defined as hemoglobin <100–110 g/L, sometimes in combination with other abnormal iron measures).22–27, 30 Two of these studies (from the United States and the United Kingdom) found no clear benefit from supplementation.24, 25 The remainder, including the large Chilean study, reported significant benefits, with RRs ranging from 0.07 (95% CI, 0.01–0.48)26 to 0.14 (95% CI, 0.09–0.20).23 However, variability in the definitions of anemia, the unknown mix of baseline risk of children enrolled, and the variation in control group rates across these studies (from 13% to 33%) limit the interpretability of the findings for U.S. populations. Overall, the studies reported rates of anemia ranging from 0% to 22% for those in the supplementation group and from 13% to 33% in the placebo group.

In addition, hemoglobin results were reported in 8 studies,24–30, 32 with small differences between groups; 3 were significant.25, 26, 28 Nine studies reported ferritin concentrations, with conflicting results.22–30, 32

Harms of Routine Iron Supplementation in Children Ages 6 to 24 Months

None of the studies of iron supplementation reported serious harms, including accidental overdose or withdrawals due to adverse events. Five studies reported on adherence to the assigned regimen and found no impact based on iron content. In some cases, however, the control group was preferred (e.g., cow's milk over fortified or unfortified formula).24, 28–31

In 1 fair-quality trial, no clinically significant adverse events thought to be related to study interventions were reported.25 No differences in rate of gastrointestinal adverse events in toddlers consuming iron-fortified milk and those consuming unfortified milk were found in a good-quality trial (2% vs. 2%; RR, 1.0 [95% CI, 0.9–11]).29 No other studies reported the incidence of gastrointestinal adverse events.

Screening for IDA

Benefits and Harms of Screening Asymptomatic Children Ages 6 to 24 Months for IDA

As in previous reviews,16, 17 no studies evaluating the benefits or harms of screening programs for asymptomatic children ages 6 to 24 months for IDA were found.

Benefits and Harms of Treatment of IDA in Children Ages 6 to 24 Months

No new studies of oral iron treatment of IDA in infants and children 6 to 24 months of age were found. Of the studies included in the previous reviews, only 1 study (n = 110) met our current criteria.35 This study was rated as poor quality due to baseline differences in age and unclear reporting of methods. Improved growth velocity and hemoglobin and ferritin levels were found, but no differences were reported in Denver Developmental Screening Test psychomotor development outcomes compared with control subjects.

No newly published studies reporting harms of iron treatment in children ages 6 to 24 months were found. One older randomized controlled trial (n = 334), published in 199136 and not included in the previous reviews, reported no differences between children receiving iron treatment and those receiving placebo in overall incidence or incidence of specific adverse events, including gastrointestinal events.

Association Between a Change in Iron Status and Improvement in Child Health Outcomes in Populations Relevant to the United States

No studies met the established criteria to evaluate an association between improvement in impaired iron status and child health outcomes in populations relevant to the United States.

One poor-quality and 2 fair-quality supplementation studies provided evidence regarding changes in iron status and measures of growth or development scale scores in children with normal iron status at baseline.22, 25, 30 Two fair-quality studies (Table 3) found statistically significant changes in iron status measures but no differences between groups in measures of weight or height after 9 and 18 months.22, 25 A poor-quality trial found large numerical (but nonsignificant) differences in hemoglobin and serum ferritin levels and a significant difference in transferrin saturation, but no differences in weight, height, or head circumference outcomes or in neurodevelopmental outcomes based on the Bayley Scales of Infant Development.30

Return to Table of Contents

As in the previous USPSTF reviews,16, 17 we found no evidence regarding the effects of routine iron supplementation in young children on diagnosis of psychomotor or neurodevelopmental delay or quality of life, and the evidence regarding developmental test scores after 3- to 12-month follow-up periods (although not clearly clinical outcomes) does not indicate important differences.23, 26, 28, 30 We found no evidence of important or clear benefit in growth outcomes, which is consistent with the findings of a recent meta-analysis of 21 randomized controlled trials that included studies from any country.18

Although the study findings are not consistent, the evidence from 10 trials of iron supplementation in children indicates no benefit in terms of incidence of IDA, anemia, or hemoglobin; the findings were inconsistent regarding incidence of iron deficiency and serum ferritin concentrations.22–32

Some of the variation in findings may have been due to inadequate sample sizes for specific outcomes. This situation is often ideal for pooling studies to gain statistical power; in this case, however, we found both clinical and methodologic heterogeneity and did not combine the studies. For example, there was important variability in the definitions of IDA, anemia, and iron deficiency (mostly unknown baseline risk of children enrolled) and wide variation in control group rates across these studies. Although the prevalence rates of iron deficiency in the United States are currently estimated at 8%,5–7 control group rates in these studies ranged from 13% to 33%, such that generalizability of these findings to the U.S. population is unclear. Studies that did find a benefit generally had higher rates of iron deficiency in the control groups compared with studies which found no benefit, suggesting that baseline risk is important in determining who will benefit from supplementation, in terms of preventing iron deficiency. An additional factor potentially contributing to variability was the use of cow's milk as a control in several studies; use of cow's milk is considered a risk factor for IDA and is advised against before 12 months of age by the American Academy of Pediatrics and the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.2, 37, 38

In addition, 1 of the largest studies included in the review that consistently found statistically significant results supporting supplementation for most hematologic values was conducted in Chile and had a high incidence of hematologic values in the control groups. This study, which initially randomized children to receive low or high-iron supplementation, broke randomization, leading to baseline differences between the groups.26

Harms of routine iron supplementation in children were rarely reported, and supplementation did not result in higher rates in studies reporting harms. Although an older meta-analysis of 28 studies (randomized controlled trials and cohort studies) found a slightly increased risk of diarrhea with iron supplementation (RR, 1.1 [95% CI, 1.0–1.2]),39 the majority of studies were conducted in developing countries, and the age of the populations ranged from 2 days to 14 years.

As in the previous reports, evidence regarding the benefits and harms of screening for IDA in children ages 6 to 24 months is absent. Similarly, we found only very limited evidence regarding the benefits and harms of IDA treatment that is generalizable to children ages 6 to 24 months in the United States. Based on this evidence, benefits were shown only for some iron status measures in the short term. Previous USPSTF reports concluded that there was no evidence regarding the relative harms of treatment. This update identified only 1 additional study, which indicated no differences between children receiving iron supplementation and placebo in the incidence of overall or specific adverse events, including gastrointestinal events.36

The potential for long-term benefit of preventing IDA in young children presumes that improvement in iron status is associated with good long-term clinical outcomes, such as normal growth and neurodevelopment. Evidence capable of showing this specific association was extremely limited and did not support a clear association between change in iron status and differences in growth or neurodevelopment.

Limitations of our report include restricting inclusion of studies published in English and studies conducted in developed countries or studies in developing countries where the population enrolled was similar to the population of the United States, particularly in terms of rates of malnutrition, hemoparasite burden, and general socioeconomic status. A number of studies of iron supplementation and treatment that were conducted in developing countries were excluded.40–46

Malnourishment, very low socioeconomic status, and/or presence of parasitic endemic diseases were common in the included populations in these studies. Also excluded were studies of iron supplementation that enrolled children aged <6 months;47–50 this population was outside the scope of the review.

Good-quality, randomized controlled trials of routine supplementation, screening programs, and treatment of IDA in children 6 to 24 months of age, with adequate sample sizes for key iron status and clinical health outcomes, are needed. Such trials should clearly report prognostic baseline characteristics of enrolled children, details of interventions, longer-term benefits (particularly developmental outcomes using appropriate neurodevelopmental tests), and harms, and the studies should use appropriate controls (i.e., not cow's milk). In addition, these studies should report neurodevelopmental diagnoses rather than test scores.

Return to Table of Contents

Expanded and better research is needed to assess the benefits and harms of routine iron supplementation and screening to prevent IDA in young children in developed countries. At present, the limited evidence indicates no benefits in growth and neurodevelopmental test scores with supplementation, and hematologic outcomes are variably affected. The benefits and harms of treatment are largely unclear, as is the association between improvement in IDA or iron deficiency and clinical outcomes.

Return to Table of Contents

Disclaimer: The investigators are solely responsible for the content and the decision to submit it for publication. The findings and conclusions in this document are those of the authors, who are responsible for its content, and do not necessarily represent the views of the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ). No statement in this report should be construed as an official position of AHRQ or of the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.

Source: This article was published online first at www.pediatrics.org on March 31, 2015.

Grant Support: Funded by AHRQ under Contract No. HHSA-290-2012-0001-5I, Task Order No. 2. This research was funded by AHRQ under a contract to support the work of the U.S. Preventive Services Task Force. Investigators worked with U.S. Preventive Services Task Force members and AHRQ staff to develop the scope, analytic framework, and Key Questions. AHRQ had no role in study selection, quality assessment, or synthesis. AHRQ staff provided project oversight, reviewed the report to assure that the analysis met methodologic standards, and distributed the draft for peer review, including representatives of professional societies and federal agencies.

Potential Conflicts of Interest: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

Requests for Single Reprints: Marian McDonagh, PharmD, Oregon Health & Science University, 3181 SW Sam Jackson Park Rd, Mail Code: BICC, Portland, OR 97239. E-mail: mcdonagh@ohsu.edu.

Return to Table of Contents

References

1. World Health Organization. Iron deficiency anaemia: assessment, prevention and control: a guide for programme managers. Available at: www.who.int/nutrition/publications/en/ida_assessment_prevention_control.pdf. Accessed February 3, 2015
2. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Recommendations to prevent and control iron deficiency in the United States. MMWR Recomm Rep. 1998;47(RR-3):1–29.
3. Griffin IJ, Abrams SA. Iron and breastfeeding. Pediatr Clin North Am. 2001;48(2):401–13.
4. McCann JC, Ames BN. An overview of evidence for a causal relation between iron deficiency during development and deficits in cognitive or behavioral function. Am J Clin Nutr. 2007;85(4):931–45.
5. Bamberg R. Occurrence and detection of iron-deficiency anemia in infants and toddlers. Clin Lab Sci. 2008;21(4):225–31.
6. Brotanek JM, Gosz J, Weitzman M, Flores G. Secular trends in the prevalence of iron deficiency among US toddlers, 1976-2002. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2008;162(4):374–81.
7. Schneider JM, Fujii ML, Lamp CL, Lönnerdal B, Dewey KG, Zidenberg-Cherr S. Anemia, iron deficiency, and iron-deficiency anemia in 12–36-mo-old children from low-income families. Am J Clin Nutr. 2005;82(6):1269–75.
8. Baker RD, Greer FR; Committee on Nutrition American Academy of Pediatrics. Diagnosis and prevention of iron deficiency and iron-deficiency anemia in infants and young children (0-3 years of age). Pediatrics. 2010;126(5):1040–50.
9. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). Iron deficiency—United States, 1999-2000. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2002;51(40):897–9.
10. Cusick SE, Mei Z, Freedman DS, et al. Unexplained decline in the prevalence of anemia among US children and women between 1988-1994 and 1999-2002. Am J Clin Nutr. 2008;88(6):1611–7.
11. Hay G, Sandstad B, Whitelaw A, Borch-Iohnsen B. Iron status in a group of Norwegian children aged 6-24 months. Acta Paediatr. 2004;93(5):592–8.
12. Oti-Boateng P, Seshadri R, Petrick S, Gibson RA, Simmer K. Iron status and dietary iron intake of 6–24-month-old children in Adelaide. J Paediatr Child Health. 1998;34(3):250–3.
13. U.S. Preventive Services Task Force. Screening for iron deficiency anemia, including iron supplementation for children and pregnant women. Rockville, MD: U.S. Preventive Services Task Force; 2006.
14. McDonagh M, Blazina I, Dana T, Cantor A, Bougatsos C. Routine Iron Supplementation and Screening for Iron Deficiency Anemia in Children Ages 6 to 24 Months: Systematic Review to Update the U.S. Preventive Services Task Force Recommendation. Evidence Synthesis No. 122. AHRQ Publication No. 13-05187-EF-1. Rockville, MD: Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality; 2015.
15. U.S. Preventive Services Task Force. Procedure Manual. 2008. Available at: http://www.uspreventiveservicestaskforce.org/Page/Name/procedure-manual. Accessed March 25, 2015.
16. Oregon Evidence-based Practice Center. Screening for iron deficiency anemia in childhood and pregnancy: update of the 1996 U.S. Preventive Services Task Force Review. Evidence Synthesis No. 40. AHRQ Publication No. 06-0590-EF-1. Rockville, MD: Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality; 2006.
17. DiGuiseppi C, ed. Screening for iron deficiency anemia—including iron prophylaxis. Guide to Clinical Preventive Services. 2nd ed. Report of the U.S. Preventive Services Task Force. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins; 1996.
18. Vucic V, Berti C, Vollhardt C, et al. Effect of iron intervention on growth during gestation, infancy, childhood, and adolescence: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Nutr Rev. 2013;71(6):386–401.
19. Logan S, Martins S, Gilbert R. Iron therapy for improving psychomotor development and cognitive function in children under the age of three with iron deficiency anaemia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2009(1):CD001444.
20. Wang B, Zhan S, Gong T, Lee L. Iron therapy for improving psychomotor development and cognitive function in children under the age of three with iron deficiency anaemia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2013(6):CD001444.
21. United Nations Development Programme, Human Development Reports. Data. Available at: http://hdr.undp.org/en/statistics/. Accessed February 3, 2015.
22. Daly A, MacDonald A, Aukett A, et al. Prevention of anaemia in inner city toddlers by an iron supplemented cows' milk formula. Arch Dis Child. 1996;75(1):9–16.
23. Williams J, Wolff A, Daly A, MacDonald A, Aukett A, Booth IW. Iron supplemented formula milk related to reduction in psychomotor decline in infants from inner city areas: randomised study. BMJ. 1999;318(7185):693–7.
24. Geltman PL, Meyers AF, Mehta SD, et al. Daily multivitamins with iron to prevent anemia in high-risk infants: a randomized clinical trial. Pediatrics. 2004;114(1):86–93.
25. Gill DG, Vincent S, Segal DS. Follow-on formula in the prevention of iron deficiency: a multicentre study. Acta Paediatr. 1997;86(7):683–9.
26. Lozoff B, De Andraca I, Castillo M, Smith JB, Walter T, Pino P. Behavioral and developmental effects of preventing iron-deficiency anemia in healthy full-term infants. Pediatrics. 2003;112(4):846–54.
27. Makrides M, Leeson R, Gibson R, Simmer K. A randomized controlled clinical trial of increased dietary iron in breast-fed infants. J Pediatr. 1998;133(4):559–62.
28. Morley R, Abbott R, Fairweather-Tait S, MacFadyen U, Stephenson T, Lucas A. Iron-fortified follow-on formula from 9 to 18 months improves iron status but not development or growth: a randomised trial. Arch Dis Child. 1999;81(3):247–52.
29. Szymlek-Gay EA, Ferguson EL, Heath AL, Gray AR, Gibson RS. Food-based strategies improve iron status in toddlers: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr. 2009;90(6):1541–51.
30. Yalçin SS, Yurdakök K, Açikgöz D, Özmert E. Short-term developmental outcome ofiron prophylaxis in infants. Pediatr Int. 2000;42(6):625–30.
31. Yeung GS, Zlotkin SH. Efficacy of meat and iron-fortified commercial cereal to prevent iron depletion in cow milk-fed infants 6 to 12 months of age: a randomized controlled trial. Can J Public Health. 2000;91(4):263–7.
32. Domellöf M, Cohen RJ, Dewey KG, Hernell O, Rivera LL, Lönnerdal B. Iron supplementation of breast-fed Honduran and Swedish infants from 4 to 9 months of age. J Pediatr. 2001;138(5):679–87.
33. Jary S, Whitelaw A, Walløe L, Thoresen M. Comparison of Bayley-2 and Bayley-3 scores at 18 months in term infants following neonatal encephalopathy and therapeutic hypothermia. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2013;55(11):1053–9.
34. Moore T, Johnson S, Haider S, Hennessy E, Marlow N. The Bayley-III cognitive and language scales: how do scores relate to the Bayley II? Arch Dis Child. 2011;96(Suppl 1):A39–40.
35. Aukett MA, Parks YA, Scott PH, Wharton BA. Treatment with iron increases weight gain and psychomotor development. Arch Dis Child. 1986;61(9):849–57.
36. Irigoyen M, Davidson LL, Carriero D, Seaman C. Randomized, placebo-controlled trial of iron supplementation in infants with low hemoglobin levels fed iron-fortified formula. Pediatrics. 1991;88(2):320–6.
37. American Academy of Pediatrics. Why formula instead of cow's milk. Available at: www.healthychildren.org/English/ages-stages/baby/feeding-nutrition/Pages/Why-Formula-Instead-of-Cows-Milk.aspx. Accessed February 3, 2015
38. American Academy of Pediatrics Committee on Nutrition. The use of whole cow's milk in infancy. Pediatrics. 1992;89(6):1105–9.
39. Gera T, Sachdev HP. Effect of iron supplementation on incidence of infectious illness in children: systematic review. BMJ. 2002;325(7373):1142.
40. Lind T, Lönnerdal B, Stenlund H, et al. A community-based randomized controlled trial of iron and zinc supplementation in Indonesian infants: effects on growth and development. Am J Clin Nutr. 2004;80(3):729–36.
41. Majumdar I, Paul P, Talib VH, Ranga S. The effect of iron therapy on the growth of iron-replete and iron-deplete children. J Trop Pediatr. 2003;49(2):84–8.
42. Pollitt E, Saco-Pollitt C, Jahari A, Husaini MA, Huang J. Effects of an energy and micronutrient supplement on mental development and behavior under natural conditions in undernourished children in Indonesia. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2000;54(Suppl 2):S80–90.
43. Olney DK, Pollitt E, Kariger PK, et al. Combined iron and folic acid supplementation with or without zinc reduces time to walking unassisted among Zanzibari infants 5- to 11-mo old. J Nutr. 2006;136(9):2427–34.
44. Villalpando S, Shamah T, Rivera JA, Lara Y, Monterrubio E. Fortifying milk with ferrous gluconate and zinc oxide in a public nutrition program reduced the prevalence of anemia in toddlers. J Nutr. 2006;136(10):2633–7.
45. Akman M, Cebeci D, Okur V, Angin H, Abali O, Akman AC. The effects of iron deficiency on infants' developmental test performance. Acta Paediatr. 2004;93(10):1391–6.
46. Engstrom EM, Castro IR, Portela M, Cardoso LO, Monteiro CA. Effectiveness of daily and weekly iron supplementation in the prevention of anemia in infants. Rev Saude Publica. 2008;42(5):786–95.
47. Abdelrazik N, Al-Haggar M, Al-Marsafawy H, Abdel-Hadi H, Al-Baz R, Mostafa AH. Impact of long-term oral ironsupplementation in breast-fed infants. Indian J Pediatr. 2007;74(8):739–45.
48. Berglund S, Westrup B, Domellöf M. Iron supplements reduce the risk of iron deficiency anemia in marginally low birth weight infants. Pediatrics. 2010;126(4).
49. Sankar MJ, Saxena R, Mani K, Agarwal R, Deorari AK, Paul VK. Early iron supplementation in very low birth weight infants—a randomized controlled trial. Acta Paediatr. 2009;98(6):953–8.
50. Ziegler EE, Nelson SE, Jeter JM. Iron supplementation of breastfed infants from an early age. Am J Clin Nutr. 2009;89(2):525–32.

Return to Table of Contents

aCochrane databases include the Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials and the Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews.
bOther sources include previous reports, reference lists of relevant articles, and systematic reviews.
cSome studies are included for >1 Key Question (KQ).
dIncludes 2 poor-quality studies.

Text Description.

This figure is a literature flow diagram depicting the search and selection of studies for the review. The diagram shows that 2,159 abstracts of potentially relevant articles were identified through the MEDLINE and Cochrane databases, 2,030 articles and background papers were excluded, 129 articles were reviewed at the full-text level, and 118 articles were excluded for the following reasons: 53 due to wrong population, 14 due to wrong intervention, 5 due to wrong outcome, 19 due to wrong study design, 5 due to wrong publication type, 13 due to wrong comparison, 8 were systematic reviews that were not directly used, and 1 due to data being used from another source. 10 unique trials (in 11 publications) were ultimately included in the review. For Key Question 1 on routine iron supplementation, 6 trials were included for growth outcomes, 4 trials were included for developmental test score outcomes, and 10 trials were included for hematological outcomes (including 2 poor-quality studies). For Key Question 2, 2 trials were included on harms of supplementation. For the section of the report on screening for iron deficiency anemia, no studies were includes for any of the Key Questions.

Return to Table of Contents

Study, Year, Country, N, Duration; Quality Risk Factors Reported Interventions and Comparator Outcomes: Supplementation vs. Control Groups
IDA (Hb <110 g/L and Iron Deficiencya) Anemia (Hb <110 g/L) Iron Deficiencya Hb Serum Ferritin Growth
Domellöf et al, 200132
Sweden
N = 70
3 mo
Fair
Race: not reported; preterm and low-birthweight infants excluded A. Placebo from 4 to 6 mo and iron supplement from 6 to 9 mo of age (n = 34)
B. Placebo from 4 to 9 mo (n = 36)
No effect (numbers not reported)     117.1 vs. 114.4 g/L; P=NS 47.3 vs. 22.9 µg/L; P<0.001 Weight at 9 mo: 8.9 vs. 8.9 kg (P=NS)
Geltman et al, 200424
United States
N = 284
3 mo
Fair
Race: 55% vs. 48% black; preterm and low-birthweight infants excluded A. Oral multivitamin drops, 10 mg of iron/d
B. Multivitamin drops without iron
8% (11/138) vs. 8% (11/144) anemic and had 2 other abnormal hematologic values; RR, 1.04 (95% CI, 0.47–2.33) 22% (31/138) vs. 19% (27/144); RR, 1.20 (95% CI, 0.76–1.90) 78% (108/138) vs. 84% (121/144) had 1 abnormal hematologic value indicative of iron deficiency; RR, 0.93 (95% CI, 0.83–1.04) 117 vs. 117 g/L; P=NS 32.0 vs. 29.2 µg/L; P=NS  
Gill et al, 199725
United Kingdom and Ireland
N = 302
11 mo
Fair
Race: not reported; preterm and low-birthweight infants excluded A. Iron-fortified formula
B. Non–iron-fortified formula
C. Cow's milk
  11% vs. 13% vs. 33%b 6% vs. 22% vs. 43%b 121.5 vs. 117.7 vs. 111.4 g/L; P=0.006c 25.1 vs. 15.3 vs. 11.0 µg/L; P<0.001 Weight: 11.1 vs. 11.1 vs. 11.3 kg (P=NS) Length: 78.9 vs. 79.1 vs. 80.3 cm (P=NS)
Lozoff et al, 200326
Chile
N = 1657
6–12 mo
Fair
Race: not reported; preterm and low-birthweight infants excluded A. Multiple interventions with varying iron concentrations
B. No iron supplementation
3.1% (34/1114) vs. 22.6% (116/514); RR, 0.14 (95% CI, 0.09–0.20) 4.3% (48/1123) vs. 25.8% (138/534); RR, 0.17 (95% CI, 0.12–0.23) 26.5% (286/1081) vs. 51.3% (273/532); RR, 0.52 (95% CI, 0.45–0.59) 123.6 vs. 115.6 g/L; P<0.001 14.0 vs. 8.7 µg/L; P<0.001 Weight: 10.0 vs. 10.1 kg (P<0.05) Weight for age (z score): 0.05 vs. 0.13 (P=NS)
Length: 74.7 vs. 75.1 cm (P<0.001) Length for age (z score): −0.27 vs. −0.15 (P<0.001)
Head circumference: 46.7 vs. 47.0 cm (P<0.001)
Makrides et al, 199827
Australia
N = 62
6 mo
Fair
Race: not reported; preterm and low-birthweight infants excluded A. High-iron weaning diet
B. Control weaning
diet
0 vs. 0 0 vs. 19.2% (5/26); RR, 0.07 (95% CI, 0.00–1.15) 3.9% (5/36) vs. 7.7% (2/26); RR, 1.81 (95% CI, 0.38–8.60) 120 vs. 115 g/L; P=NS 26 vs. 35 µg/L; P=NS  
Morley et al, 199928
United Kingdom
N = 428
9 mo
Fair
Race: not reported; preterm and low-birthweight infants excluded A. Iron-fortified formula, 1.2 mg iron/L
B. Unfortified formula, 0.9 mg iron/L
C. Cow's milk
      126 vs. 120 vs. 119 g/L; P=0.01 for A vs. C, P=0.05 for A vs. B 21.7 vs. 13.1 vs. 14.3 µg/L; P=0.0001 for A vs. B and A vs. C Weight: 11.4 vs. 11.3 vs. 11.4 kg (P=NS)
Length: 82.3 vs. 82.3 vs. 82.6 cm (P=NS)
Szymlek-Gay et al, 200929
New Zealand
N = 225
5 mo
Good
Race: 84% vs. 78% vs. 76% white A. $2 portions of red meat, 1.3 mg of iron per portion
B. Iron-fortified milk, 0.5 mg iron/100 g
C. Cow's milk
No difference between groups (data only reported in a figure)     118.6 vs. 121.5 vs. 120.2 g/L; P=NS 32.8 vs. 43.5 vs. 29.9 µg/L; effect size in fortified milk group, 1.68 (95% CI, 1.27–2.24) No effect on growth
Williams et al, 199923, other publication: Daly et al, 199622
United Kingdom
N = 92
10–12 mo
Fair
Race: 74% white, 24% black, 2% Asian
Receiving income support: 59%
Preterm and low-birthweight infants excluded
A. Iron-supplemented formula, 1.2 mg iron/100 mL
B. Cow's milk
  At 18 mo of age: 2% (1/46) vs. 33% (15/46); RR, 0.07 (95% CI, 0.01 to 0.48)     30.5 vs. 15.9 µg/L; P=0.001 No effect on growth (data not shown)

Two trials were omitted from the table due to poor-quality rating (Yalçin et al, 200030 and Yeung and Zlotkin, 200031).
Hb = hemoglobin; NS = not significant.
a Iron deficiency was defined variably as serum ferritin <10 µg/L,25, 27, 30 <15 µg/L,24 or <12 µg/L in addition to 1 of 2 other hematologic indicators.26
b RR not calculable based on data reported.
c Groups differed significantly at baseline.

Return to Table of Contents

Study, Year, Country, N, Quality Duration Risk Factors Reported Interventions and Comparator Mental Development Index Psychomotor Development Index
Lozoff et al, 200326
Chile
N = 1657
Fair
6 mo Race: not reported; preterm and low-birthweight infants excluded A. Multiple interventions with varying iron concentrations
B. No iron supplementation
103.9 vs. 104.6; P=NS 96.7 vs. 97.5; P=NS
Morley et al, 199928
United Kingdom
N = 428
Fair
9 mo Race: not reported; preterm and low-birthweight infants excluded A. Iron-fortified formula, 1.2 mg iron/L
B. Unfortified formula, 0.9 mg iron/L
C. Cow's milk
93.9 vs. 94.5 vs. 96.2; P=NS 94.8 vs. 94.6 vs. 93.6; P=NS

One trial omitted from table due to poor-quality rating (Yalçin et al, 200030).
NS = not significant.

Return to Table of Contents

Study, Year, Country, N, Quality Duration Intervention Groups Baseline Iron Status Follow-Up Iron Status Mean Change in Iron Status Health Outcomes
Daly et al, 199622
United Kingdom
N = 100
Fair
18 mo A. Iron-fortified formula (n = 41)
B. Cow's milk (n = 43)
A vs. B
Hemoglobin: 119 vs. 120 g/L
Serum ferritin: 33.2 vs. 34.6 µg/L
A vs. B
Hemoglobin: 124 vs. 118 g/L
Serum ferritin: 32.4 vs. 14.9 µg/L
A vs. B
Hemoglobin: 5.0 vs. −2.0 g/L (P=0.0001)
Serum ferritin: 0.8 vs. −19.7 µg/L (P<0.001)
A vs. B
No difference between groups in weight for age, weight for height, or height for age (data not shown)
Gill et al, 199725
United Kingdom and Ireland
N = 302
Fair
9 mo A. Iron-fortified formula (n = 264)
B. Formula without iron fortification (n = 85)
C. Cow's milk (n = 57)
A vs. B vs. C
Hemoglobin: 118.9 vs. 116.6 vs. 114.4 µg/L
Serum ferritin: 41.6 vs. 42.3 vs. 31.9 µg/L
Serum iron: 14.9 vs. 13.9 vs. 12.5 mmol/L
Total iron-binding capacity: 61.1 vs. 59.0 vs. 64.9 mmol/L
A vs. B vs. C
Hemoglobin: 121.5 vs. 117.7 vs. 111.4 µg/L
Serum ferritin: 25.1 vs. 15.3 vs. 11.0 µg/L
Serum iron: 14.4 vs. 12.9 vs. 10.0 mol/L
Total iron-binding capacity: 63.0 vs. 70.3 vs. 73.2 mmol/L
A vs. B vs. C
Hemoglobin: 2.6 vs. 1.1 vs. −3.0 µg/L (A vs. B and C, P<0.001)
Serum ferritin: −16.5 vs. −27.0 vs. −20.9 µg/L (A vs. B and C, P<0.001)
Serum iron: −0.5 vs. −1.0 vs. −2.5 mmol/L (A vs. B and C, P=0.04)
Total iron binding
capacity: 1.9 vs. 11.3 vs. 8.3 mmol/L (A vs. B and C, P=0.05)
A vs. B vs. C
Weight, mean change from baseline: 11.1 (3.2) vs. 11.1 (3.1) vs. 11.3 (3.0) kg (P=NS for all comparisons)
Length, mean change from baseline: 79.1 (11.3) vs. 78.9 (11.5) vs. 80.3 (12.2) cm (P=NS for all comparisons)

One trial omitted from table due to poor-quality rating (Yalçin et al, 200030).
NS = not significant.

Return to Table of Contents